Publications
Do We Have to Give Up?
No. We Know How to Get Smart About IQ
By Douglas J. Besharov
This article originally appeared in The Washington Post, October 23, 1994.
IT SEEMED like a simple enough project. Christmas was coming and
the local mall had jobs for gift wrappers. What better way for mothers on
welfare to earn a few extra dollars? So a local job training program decided to
give a group of welfare mothers a quick course in gift-wrapping before sending
them off to apply for a job. It wasn't that easy.
The first lesson was bows. The instructor asked the mothers to cut
pieces of ribbon, each five inches long. The mothers quickly became confused --
they did not know how to measure off the ribbon for cutting. There would be no
jobs at the mall that season, because the mothers lacked the basic cognitive
skills to wrap packages.
This true story illustrates a harsh reality: Long-term welfare
recipients have extremely low cognitive abilities, at least as measured by
traditional IQ tests. This is true for all races -- the women at the training
center just happened to be white. Almost 60 percent of women on welfare for five
or more years are in the bottom 20 percent of intelligence, according to Richard
Herrnstein and Charles Murray, authors of the controversial new book "The Bell
Curve."
Herrnstein and Murray air social science's dirty little secret: IQ
matters. As they document, using data for whites, high IQ is increasingly
associated with economic and social success -- and low IQ with poverty and a
host of social problems, including out-of-wedlock births, welfare dependency and
crime. This undeniable reality is stunningly ignored by just about every program
designed to address such problems. No wonder they fail.
"The Bell Curve's" unflinching recognition of racial differences
in IQ test scores has, of course, generated the greatest controversy. African
Americans, as a group, consistently score 15 points below whites, 85 versus 100.
According to the authors, "The average white person tests higher than about 84
percent of the population of blacks."
But, what, exactly, is IQ? And, how much of what we call IQ is
attributable to inborn qualities and how much to environment and upbringing?
This is, of course, the "nature" vs. "nurture" argument, which has been with us
since before there were IQ tests. Reflecting the current scholarly consensus,
Herrnstein and Murray say that "cognitive ability is substantially heritable,
apparently no less than 40 percent and no more than 80 percent." For the
purposes of their argument, they adopt a mid-range estimate of 60 percent
heritability.
Especially in light of black-white differences in measured IQ, the
issue of heritability is enormously significant. In response to charges that the
book overstates the genetic component of IQ, Murray has recently written that it
does not matter whether nature or nurture causes IQ differences, because, either
way, IQ is so difficult to raise. As evidence, Murray cites the failure of
compensatory preschool and educational programs to raise IQs and to make
meaningful changes in young people's lives. No clear-eyed reader of the research
literature could deny these disappointing results.
But such programs hardly exhaust the possible interventions. Many
linkages may exist between an individual's environment and his or her
subsequently measured IQ, and these offer opportunities for intervention. Here
are a few possibilities: What if IQ is affected by the mother's behavior during
pregnancy?
In recent years, science has documented the importance of the
fetal environment to later development. The message we try to give every
pregnant woman is: "Eat well, don't smoke cigarettes, don't drink alcohol and,
most importantly, don't use illegal drugs like crack cocaine." Prenatal exposure
to cocaine results in newborns with smaller head circumferences, a sign of
compromised brain development.
All of these harmful behaviors are far more widespread among
disadvantaged mothers. But they seem to afflict some racial minorities even more
than economic statistics would suggest. In a recent survey, for example,
Hispanic women were almost twice as likely to use cocaine while pregnant than
were white women; African-American women were 11 times more likely to use
cocaine. What if the first years of life are crucial?
In the first months of life, the number of synapses in the human
brain increases twentyfold, from 50 trillion to 1,000 trillion. The absence of
intellectual stimulation during this period is now believed to impose a
permanent limit on the number of synapses and, therefore, on intellectual
potential. This phenomenon was demonstrated in a famous experiment in which the
eyes of newborn kittens were covered for varying lengths of time. The longer
their eyes were covered, the greater the permanent deficit in sight, not because
their eyes were damaged, but because there were just fewer synapses in the areas
of the brain responsible for processing visual images. As Jerry M. Wiener,
chairman of the Department of Psychiatry at George Washington University
Hospital and president of the American Psychiatric Association, explains: "What
we call IQ is really the unfolding of innate abilities in response to
environmental stimuli." Even smiling at a newborn may make a difference.
Again, it is disadvantaged families that are least likely to
provide such cognitive cues. Numerous studies, for example, have described
"lower-class child-rearing styles" as more angry and punitive, more humiliating,
and less verbally interactive than middle-class child-rearing -- and have
correlated these differences to cognitive outcomes.
These developmentally stunting child-rearing styles tend to
disappear as families become more middle-class, but, as Felton Earls, director
of the Human Development and Criminal Behavior Program at Harvard University,
explains, it can take two or three generations for the shift to occur. With so
many African Americans only recently in the middle class, it should not be
surprising that such behaviors have tended to linger on in what researchers
consider middle-class households. What if preschool interventions could make a
real change in a child's learning environment?
The Abecedarian preschool project in Chapel Hill, N.C., seems to
have raised IQ scores by 16 points at the end of three years. Unlike Head Start,
the Abecedarian project totally immersed children in a comprehensive
developmental program that began within three months of birth -- and provided
nearly full-time care until they reached school age. Unfortunately, as the
children got older -- and they spent progressively less time under the influence
of program staff -- the gap between the experimental and control groups
narrowed, to 7.6 IQ points at age 5, and 4.6 points at age 15.
"Other preschool projects have also made improvements of 10 or
more IQ points," notes Ron Haskins, who was the coordinator of the Abecedarian
preschool project in the late 1970s, and is now the chief welfare specialist for
House Republicans. "In all these projects, however, the initial IQ gains for the
children in the program compared to those in the control group also shrank over
time."
So, we seem to be able to make early improvements in IQ; we just
don't know how to make them stick. Some argue that this is the infamous
"fade-out effect," with the children in the control group catching up with those
in the program. It is just as possible, however, that other environmental
factors, like neighborhood, had a supervening effect on the children in the
program. What if good schools raise scores?
Forget about cultural bias in IQ tests. There is a bigger
measurement problem. Most tests assess acquired knowledge as well as abstract
thinking and problem solving, and that, of course, is where schools come in. A
poor school environment, where discipline rather than learning is the first
priority, could systematically depress test results. After all, if education
does not matter, why are we so concerned about the quality of the schools where
we send our children?
A change in the way schools teach could also narrow the
black-white gap, according to Chester Finn, the founding partner and director of
government relations for the Edison Project. "Conventional schools assume that
all children learn one grade level a year, so they give both slow learners and
quick learners the same 180 days of education. What if schools gave slow
learners more time to learn? Would they do better? We cannot know until we try."
What if neighborhoods dampen the desire of children to perform well?
Young people are particularly sensitive to environmental
influences. Sadly, many disadvantaged communities discourage intellectual
achievement. In African American communities, some good students are ostracized
for "acting white." That's one reason why so many of the parents who can do so
move away from dysfunctional neighborhoods, and why so many of the parents who
cannot leave do all they can to shield their children from neighborhood
influences. Linda Burton, now a professor in human development at Pennsylvania
State University, describes how she and her sisters were locked in their
apartment after school to protect them from what was happening outside -- and
how the practice continues to this day in many inner-city neighborhoods. What if
a child's entire neighborhood environment is improved dramatically?
We have a tantalizing suggestion from Chicago, where, as the
result of the settlement of a housing discrimination lawsuit, Gautraux v. the
U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development Chicago Housing Authority,
individual black families from housing projects could choose to participate in a
program in which they would, by random assignment, be moved to either suburban
white middle-class neighborhoods or to middle-class black ones within the city.
Ninety-five percent of the black children who grew up in the suburbs graduated
from high school and 54 percent went on to college, compared to 80 percent and
21 percent, respectively, of those who remained in the city. What if racial
differences in IQ are the result of over 200 years of slavery and more than 100
more years of discrimination and oppression?
Since the 1950s, of course, black Americans have made major
economic progress. Earnings for black men, for example, are now about 75 percent
of those for white men, and the gap continues to close. But the figures for
household wealth paint a much more dismal picture and show how far behind whites
blacks still are: According to the Census Bureau, in 1988, the median net worth
of white households was 10 times that of black and Hispanic ones, about $ 43,000
compared to about $ 4,000 and about $ 5,500, respectively.
But household wealth is not just money. It is also a form of
stored human capital that has been built up over generations. It is what Roger
Wilkins, the Robinson professor of history and American culture at George Mason
University, describes as "the accumulated ease in dealing with the wider
society." These stark disparities give a sense of the remaining gap in human
capital between the races. Might not this legacy take many generations to erase?
If so, perhaps carefully targeted education and affirmative action programs
could succeed in giving this generation a jump start toward equality -- thus
permitting the next generation to reach its potential. Herrnstein and Murray are
right in saying that there is no proven way to raise IQs on a consistent basis.
Indeed, doing something about the environmental aspects of low cognitive ability
may be more difficult than any of us suppose and raises troubling questions
about parental behaviors and the performance of public institutions. But just
asking these questions demonstrates why identifying the origins of IQ
differences is so important.
If IQ matters as much as it seems to, and if IQ is substantially
affected by the environment or, as is more likely, is the result of a complex
interaction between genes and environment, then we are morally bound to keep
plugging away until we find something that works to raise it.
Douglas Besharov, a resident scholar at the American Enterprise
Institute, was the first director of the U.S. National Center on Child Abuse and
Neglect. His most recent book is Recognizing Child Abuse: A Guide for the
Concerned.
(For a PDF version, please click here.)
Back to top